Part 1
From a number of
recent studies, it has become clear that blind people can appreciate the use of
outlines and perspectives to describe the arrangement of objects and other
surfaces in space.
But pictures are
more than literal representations. This fact was drawn to my attention
dramatically when a blind woman in one of my investigations decided on her own
initiative to draw a wheel as it was spinning. To show this motion, she traced
a curve inside the circle (Fig. 1). I was taken aback, lines of motion, such as
the one she used, are a very recent invention in the history of illustration.
Indeed, as art scholar David Kunzle notes, Wilhelm Busch, a trend-setting
nineteenth-century cartoonist, used virtually no motion lines in his popular
figure until about 1877.
When I asked several other blind study subjects to draw a spinning wheel, one particularly clever rendition appeared repeatedly: several subjects showed the wheel's spokes as curves lines. When asked about these curves, they all described them as metaphorical ways of suggesting motion. Majority rule would argue that this device somehow indicated motion very well. But was it a better indicator than, say, broken or wavy lines-or any other kind of line, for that matter? The answer was not clear. So I decided to test whether various lines of motion were apt ways of showing movement or if they were merely idiosyncratic marks. Moreover, I wanted to discover whether there were differences in how the blind and the sighted interpreted lines of motion.
To search out these answers, I created raised-line drawings of five different wheels, depicting spokes with lines that curved, bent, waved, dashed and extended beyond the perimeters of the wheel. I then asked eighteen blind volunteers to feel the wheels and assign one of the following motions to each wheel: wobbling, spinning fast, spinning steadily, jerking or braking. My control group consisted of eighteen sighted undergraduates from the University of Toronto.
When I asked several other blind study subjects to draw a spinning wheel, one particularly clever rendition appeared repeatedly: several subjects showed the wheel's spokes as curves lines. When asked about these curves, they all described them as metaphorical ways of suggesting motion. Majority rule would argue that this device somehow indicated motion very well. But was it a better indicator than, say, broken or wavy lines-or any other kind of line, for that matter? The answer was not clear. So I decided to test whether various lines of motion were apt ways of showing movement or if they were merely idiosyncratic marks. Moreover, I wanted to discover whether there were differences in how the blind and the sighted interpreted lines of motion.
To search out these answers, I created raised-line drawings of five different wheels, depicting spokes with lines that curved, bent, waved, dashed and extended beyond the perimeters of the wheel. I then asked eighteen blind volunteers to feel the wheels and assign one of the following motions to each wheel: wobbling, spinning fast, spinning steadily, jerking or braking. My control group consisted of eighteen sighted undergraduates from the University of Toronto.
Words associated
with circle/square
SOFT-HARD
MOTHER-FATHER
HAPPY-SAD
GOOD-EVIL
LOVE-HATE
ALIVE-DEAD
BRIGHT-DARK
LIGHT-HEAVY
WARM-COLD
SUMMER-WINTER
WEAK-STRONG
FAST-SLOW
CAT-DOG
SPRING-FALL
QUIET-LOUD
WALKING-STANDING
ODD-EVEN
FAR-NEAR
PLANT-ANIMAL
DEEP-SHALLOW
|
Agreement among
subjects(%)
100
94
94
89
89
87
87
85
81
81
79
79
74
74
62
62
57
53
53
51
|
All but one of the
blind subjects assigned distinctive motions to each wheel. Most guessed that
the curved spokes indicated that the wheel was spinning steadily; the wavy
spokes, they thought; suggested that the wheel was wobbling; and the bent
spokes were taken as a sign that the wheel was jerking. Subjects assumed that
spokes extending beyond the wheel's perimeter signified that the wheel had its
brakes on and that dashed spokes indicated the wheel was spinning quickly.
In addition, the favored description for the sighted was favored description for the blind in every instance. What is more, the consensus among the sighted was barely higher than that among the blind. Because motion devices are unfamiliar to the blind, the task I gave them involved some problem solving. Evidently, however, the blind not only figured out meaning for each of motion, but as a group they generally came up with the same meaning at least as frequently as did sighted subjects.
Part 2
We have found that the blind understand other kinds of visual metaphors as well. One blind woman drew a picture of a child inside a heart-choosing that symbol, she said, to show that love surrounded the child. With Chang Hong Liu, a doctoral student from china, I have begun exploring how well blind people understand the symbolism behind shapes such as hearts that do not directly represent their meaning.
We gave a list of twenty pairs of words to sighted subjects and asked them to pick from each pair the term that best related to a circle and the term that best related to assure. For example, we asked: what goes with soft? A circle or a square? Which shapes goes with hard?
All our subjects deemed the circle soft and the square hard. A full 94% ascribed happy to the circle, instead of sad. But other pairs revealed less agreement: 79% matched fast to slow and weak to strong, respectively. And only 51% linked deep to circle and shallow to square. (see Fig. 2) When we tested four totally blind volunteers using the same list, we found that their choices closely resembled those made by he sighted subjects. One man, who had been blind since birth, scored extremely well. He made only one match differing from the consensus, assigning 'far' to square and 'near' to circle. In fact, only a small majority of sighted subjects-53%- had paired far and near to the opposite partners. Thus we concluded that the blind interpret abstract shapes as sighted people do.
In addition, the favored description for the sighted was favored description for the blind in every instance. What is more, the consensus among the sighted was barely higher than that among the blind. Because motion devices are unfamiliar to the blind, the task I gave them involved some problem solving. Evidently, however, the blind not only figured out meaning for each of motion, but as a group they generally came up with the same meaning at least as frequently as did sighted subjects.
Part 2
We have found that the blind understand other kinds of visual metaphors as well. One blind woman drew a picture of a child inside a heart-choosing that symbol, she said, to show that love surrounded the child. With Chang Hong Liu, a doctoral student from china, I have begun exploring how well blind people understand the symbolism behind shapes such as hearts that do not directly represent their meaning.
We gave a list of twenty pairs of words to sighted subjects and asked them to pick from each pair the term that best related to a circle and the term that best related to assure. For example, we asked: what goes with soft? A circle or a square? Which shapes goes with hard?
All our subjects deemed the circle soft and the square hard. A full 94% ascribed happy to the circle, instead of sad. But other pairs revealed less agreement: 79% matched fast to slow and weak to strong, respectively. And only 51% linked deep to circle and shallow to square. (see Fig. 2) When we tested four totally blind volunteers using the same list, we found that their choices closely resembled those made by he sighted subjects. One man, who had been blind since birth, scored extremely well. He made only one match differing from the consensus, assigning 'far' to square and 'near' to circle. In fact, only a small majority of sighted subjects-53%- had paired far and near to the opposite partners. Thus we concluded that the blind interpret abstract shapes as sighted people do.
Questions 27-29
Choose the correct letter, A, B,C or D.
Write your answers in boxes 27-29 on your answer sheet.
27. In the first paragraph the writer makes the point that blind people
A. may be interested in studying art.
B. can draw outlines of different objects and surfaces.
C. can recognize conventions such as perspective.
D. can draw accurately.
Choose the correct letter, A, B,C or D.
Write your answers in boxes 27-29 on your answer sheet.
27. In the first paragraph the writer makes the point that blind people
A. may be interested in studying art.
B. can draw outlines of different objects and surfaces.
C. can recognize conventions such as perspective.
D. can draw accurately.
28. The writer was surprised because the blind
woman
A. drew a circle on her own initiative.
B. did not understand what a wheel looked like.
C. included a symbol representing movement.
D. was the first person to use lines of motion.
A. drew a circle on her own initiative.
B. did not understand what a wheel looked like.
C. included a symbol representing movement.
D. was the first person to use lines of motion.
29. From the experiment described in Part 1,the
writer found that the blind subjects
A. had good understanding of symbols representing movement.
B. could control the movement of wheels very accurately.
C. worked together well as a group in solving problems.
D. got better results than the sighted undergraduates.
A. had good understanding of symbols representing movement.
B. could control the movement of wheels very accurately.
C. worked together well as a group in solving problems.
D. got better results than the sighted undergraduates.
Questions 30–32
Look at the following diagrams (Questions 30 –32), and the list of types of movement below. Match each diagram to the type of movement A–E generally assigned to it in the experiment. Choose the correct letter A–E and write them in boxes 30–32 on your answer sheet.
A steady
spinning
B jerky
movement
C rapid
spinning
D wobbling
movement
E use
of brakes
|
Questions 33–39
Complete the
summary below using words from the box. Write your answers in boxes 33-39 on your answer
sheet. NB You may use any word more than once.
In the experiment
described in Part 2, a set of word 33.......…… was
used to investigate whether blind and sighted people perceived the symbolism in
abstract 34.....…...… in
the same way. Subjects were asked which word fitted best with a circle and which
with a square. From the 35...…...… volunteers,
everyone thought a circle fitted ‘soft ’while a square fitted ‘hard’. However,
only 51% of the 36.......…… volunteers
assigned a circle to 37.....…… .When
the test was later repeated with 38...…...… volunteers,
it was found that they made 39...…...… choices.
associations blind deep hard hundred identical pairs shapes
Sighted
similar shallow soft words
|
Question 40
Choose the correct letter, A , B ,
C or D. Write your answer in box 40 on your answer sheet.
Which of the following statements best
summarizes the writer ’s general conclusion?
A The blind represent some aspects of reality differently from sighted people.
B The blind comprehend visual metaphors in similar ways to sighted people.
C The blind may create unusual and effective symbols to represent reality.
D The blind may be successful artists if given the right training.
A The blind represent some aspects of reality differently from sighted people.
B The blind comprehend visual metaphors in similar ways to sighted people.
C The blind may create unusual and effective symbols to represent reality.
D The blind may be successful artists if given the right training.
Click the Line to Show/Hide Answers
- 27. C
- 28. C
- 29. A
- 30. use of breaks
- 31. rapid spinning
- 32. steady spinning
- 33. pairs
- 34. shapes
- 35. sighted
- 36. sighted
- 37. deep
- 38. blind
- 39. similar
- 40. B
0 comment:
Post a Comment